Non-linear Dynamics and Chaotic Trajectories in Brain-Mind Visual Experiences during Dreams, Meditation, and Non-Ordinary Brain Activity States
Tania Re 1, 2, Giuseppe Vitiello 3, *
- UNESCO Chair “Anthropology of Health. Biosphere and Healing System”, University of Genoa , Genoa, Italy
- Referring Center for Phytotherapy, Tuscany Region, Careggi University Hospital , Florence, Italy
- Dipartimento di Fisica “E.R. Caianiello”, Università di Salerno, 84084 Fisciano (Salerno), Italy
* Correspondence: Giuseppe Vitiello
Academic Editor: Bart Ellenbroek
Special Issue: Quantum Brain Dynamics
Received: April 30, 2020 | Accepted: June 01, 2020 | Published: June 11, 2020
OBM Neurobiology 2020, Volume 4, Issue 2, doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2002061
Recommended citation: Re T, Vitiello G. Non-linear Dynamics and Chaotic Trajectories in Brain-Mind Visual Experiences during Dreams, Meditation, and Non-Ordinary Brain Activity States. OBM Neurobiology 2020; 4(2): 061; doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2002061.
© 2020 by the authors. This is an open access article distributed under the conditions of the Creative Commons by Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is correctly cited.
Abstract
The present report discusses brain visual experiences in conditions of low degree of openness of the brain toward the environment, for example, while dreaming, during meditation, or in non-ordinary brain activity states such as under the effects of psychoactive substances, in the state of coma, or in other states of reduced sensory perception, among others. In the present report, for brevity, such states are referred to as brain-mind visual experiences, implying that such a visual activity is not one connected to the actual vision as in the state of wakefulness. In the dissipative many-body model, the criticality of the dynamics is enhanced in low openness brain states and is at the origin of movie-like sequences of images in visual experiences. These sequences and the abrupt shifts from one image pattern to another are depicted by chaotic trajectories through the memory space. Truthfulness and realism felt in the visual experiences are discussed in terms of the algebra of the doubling of the degrees of freedom in the dissipative model. In the present discussion, a few aspects of the visual experiences of a subject during an Amazonian Ayahuasca ceremony are considered.
Keywords
Brain-mind visual experiences; dissipative quantum model of brain; memory states; chaotic trajectories; quantum field theory; models of cortical dynamics in perception; cognitive behavior
1. Introduction
The research on the dynamical laws underlying the rich phenomenology of biology has strengthened the interplay between physics and biology during the fifties and sixties of the past century. The research work conducted by Ilya Prigogine [1], and Herbert Fröhlich [2] pioneered the study of the role of dissipative systems and coherent boson condensation in biological systems through the use of the formalism of statistical mechanics, non-linear dynamical systems, and quantum field theory (QFT).
It was in this context that Ricciardi and Umezawa [3] proposed to study the neuronal dynamics within the mathematical frame of many-body physics. Early research works reported by Karl Lashley [4] and the subsequent ones by Karl Pribram [5] and Walter Freeman [6] were indeed indicating the necessity to supplement the studies focused on the properties of the single neuron and glia cell with the dynamical concepts of fields and many-body collective modes. In this context, the model developed by Ricciardi and Umezawa was extended by one of the authors (GV) to include the dissipative character of the functional activity of the brain [7].
Within the frame of the dissipative model, the aim of the present report is to account for the experiences of movie-like sequences of images in the functional activity of the brain, for examples in dreams or dream-like and sleeping states [8], during meditation, under non-ordinary states induced by psychoactive molecules (for example, DMT, i.e., Dimethyltryptamine, or N,N-DMT [9,10,11] which is closely related to serotonin, the neurotransmitter affected by a wide variety of psychedelics), perhaps under anesthesia, in certain coma states, etc. It is noteworthy, however, that the analysis presented in the present report is focused on the dynamical features that are common to the brain states during the above-stated conditions (dreams, meditation, anesthesia, etc.), and does not focus particularly on, for instance, dream activity, meditative activity, etc., nor is it focused on brain pharmacology. The interest is rather in the general dynamical mechanisms allowing these brain activities, i.e., the dynamical core shared by all these brain activities, despite their differences.
The present work demonstrates that the enhancement of the criticality of the dynamics in the low openness states of the brain is responsible for the movie-like sequences of images, scenarios, and events. Such experiences are globally referred to as brain-mind visual experiences for brevity. The locution ‘brain-mind’ is used to recall that the visual activity being referred to is not the one connected to the actual vision as in the state of wakefulness.
The dissipative model offers the unique possibility of working in the space of memory states and considering the trajectories in such a space depicting the movie-like sequences of the images in the visual experiences. The model is able to account for laboratories observations, such as the formation of extended assemblies of neurons in the synchronous amplitude-modulated and phase-modulated oscillations, their irreversible sequences, duration, and size, the fractal self-similarity of the brain background activity, the power-law distribution of power spectral densities, etc. [6,7,12,13,14,15].
The present report is organized in the following manner. The dynamical approach to brain-mind visual experiences is presented in Sections II and III, wherein criticality, openness, and deterministic chaos in the complex brain activity are also discussed. Section IV discusses intentionality and the formation of meanings within the action-perception cycle, along with the truthfulness and realism aspects felt during the brain-mind visual experiences. Section V presents conclusive remarks. The main aspects of the dissipative model are summarized in Appendix A. The present discussion also refers to a few aspects of narration by a subject who participated in an Ayahuasca ceremony in the Amazon forest. This narration is presented in Appendix B.
2. Criticality and Openness of Brain Dynamics
It might be useful, to begin with a few essential features of the dissipative quantum model of the brain. Further details are provided in Appendix A.
The brain is permanently open to its environment, exchanging energy and information with it through the perceptive channels. Inputs reaching the brain produce, through the action-perception cycle [13,16], responses that are aimed at the best being-in-the-world of the subject, i.e. to reach the equilibrium through the balancing of the fluxes of energy, information, etc. The brain identifies in the environment, the sources, and the sinks for its energy requirements and waste, respectively. The environment, therefore, appears to be its complement in the in/out (time) mirroring of fluxes, its Double. The description of the system and its environment as a closed whole implies doubling of the degrees of freedom of the system, i.e., $A_{\rm k}^{}:A_{\rm k}^{}\to A_{\rm k}^{}\times \widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$, where $\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ denotes the degrees of freedom of the environment and ${\rm k}$ denotes the momentum ($A_{\rm k}^{} and \widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ actually describe the dipole correlation modes and their time-reversed copies, respectively; cf. Appendix A).
A distinctive feature of QFT, and consequently of the dissipative model, is the existence of an infinite number of state spaces for the system, each one being physically distinct from [and inequivalent to] the others. It is possible to record memory in the minimum energy state (the vacuum or ground state) of each of these state spaces. Memory recording is achieved through a process of condensation of the quanta $A_{\rm k}^{}$ and $\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ in the vacuum, which appears to be a coherent state with a definite fractal dimension [17]. Since $A_{\rm k}^{}$ and $\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ describe the dipole correlation modes, the memories are described as correlation patterns (ordered patterns). The collection N of the numbers $N_{A_{\rm k}^{}}^{}$ and $N_{\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}}^{}$, [for any ${\rm k},N=\lbrace N_{A_{\rm k}^{}}^{},N_{\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}}^{},{\rm with}\ N_{A_{\rm k}^{}}^{}=N_{\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}}^{},\forall{\rm k} \rbrace]$, is the specific code of each memory (N is referred to as the memory order parameter). Memory “recalling” is achieved through the excitation of these quanta from the vacuum.
The use of EEG, ECoG, fNMR, and other techniques in neuroscience has revealed the formation of assemblies of myriads of neurons undergoing synchronous amplitude-modulated (AM) and phase-modulated (PM) oscillations. These AM and PM oscillation patterns are described in the model as coherent condensation patterns of neuronal long-range correlations in the brain ground state. Cortical activity is observed to go through these “multiple spatial patterns in sequences during each perceptual action that resemble cinematographic frames on multiple screens” [18,19,20].
Establishing links with the environment transforms into a dialogue between the self and the Double in a dialectical process of identification vs. distinction. It has been proposed that the act of consciousness resides in such a dialogue with the Double [7,21].
It has been shown [22] that, as the number n of the links increases (or decreases), the allowed size for the correlation domains increases (or decreases). This is consistent with the observations demonstrating that the more the brain associates with its environment, the more are the neuronal connections formed [23].
The physical inequivalence (orthogonality) among the memory states ensures that the corresponding [different] memories do not interfere with each other, and any “confusion” among these memories cannot arise. One may demonstrate that orthogonality is strict when the number of links n is maximum. Then, a phenomena such as “fixation” or “being trapped” in a certain specific memory state (in one attractor in the attractor landscape; cf. Appendix A) may occur. However, in practice, it is quite difficult to reach the maximum number of links with the environment. A higher or lower degree of openness may be reached according to several factors, occasional (such as sleeping, drug consumption, meditation, etc.) or the ones due to the age (such as during the childhood or the older ages).
On the contrary, for reduced openness, the model predicts smaller memory domains and a ‘smoothing’ of the physical inequivalence among the memory states along with an enhanced possibility of phase transitions (criticality). Then, the “paths” or trajectories through the memories may occur, producing “association” of the memories, or in certain cases “confusion” of memories. Although, in a dynamical regime of criticality, minimization of free energy at each time t is continuously pursued [7].
In a ‘normal’ state of openness, the possibility of memory flows is allowed only up to a certain degree. This is the state of “attention”, of being open to what is occurring around us, the awareness of the Now (the warning inside the Metro reads: “please pay attention, mind the gap!”).
Therefore, it is concluded in the present report that, when the number of links becomes minute such that the system almost fails to connect with the environment and is almost closed on to itself, an interrupted “flow” of memories may occur, and it becomes possible to “travel” through the memory states; the dynamics is almost completely dominated by criticality and movie-like sequences or abrupt shifting of images may be experienced.
3. Deterministic Chaos in the Complex Brain Activity
In the cases of extremely reduced openness considered earlier, the association or confusion of memories might lead to deformation or corruption of the memory code components $N_{A_{\rm k}^{}}^{}(and\ N_{\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}}^{})$, for few or several ${\rm k}$s, resulting in “pieces", “bits”, or “debris” of memories [7,21], which might be recalled in movie-like sequences, outside of their original recording context and assembled in certain emerging/new contexts.
Flows of images associated to such memory debris are observed to occur in dreams [7,21,22,24,25,26,27], in certain altered states of the brain dynamical regime such as under the effect of anesthesia [28], and in certain stages of deep meditation [29,30]; this may also occur in association with slow breathing techniques [31] or whenever the openness is indeed reduced such as in response to psychoactive substances (see the narration reported in Appendix B). Such visual experiences may also occur under the influence of external rhythmically modulated stimuli, in space (visual rhythms) or in time (musical rhythms). The repetitive persistence of these stimuli may become dominant to the point of excluding any other different input, thereby reducing the openness (similar to what happens during hypnosis).
Interestingly, in the present work we find that there exist common dynamical features underlying the different conditions in which the brain-mind visual experiences occur. Although these conditions refer to distinctly different phenomena and behaviors (ranging from dreaming to meditation, anesthesia, psychoactive substances, among others), all of these conditions are characterized by a low level of openness of the brain toward its environment, which in turn implies the criticality of the dynamics, as discussed in the previous section. The additional result that is derived from the model is that the weak inputs or noisy perturbations drive the system through the memory states and, therefore, play an important role in the complex brain activity, in general as well as particularly in the brain-mind visual experiences.
In this context, it is being remarked that deterministic chaos is a crucial feature of neuronal dynamics [12,14,32,33,34,35,36]. As stressed by Freeman, “The chaos is evident in the tendency of vast collections of neurons to shift abruptly and simultaneously from one complex activity pattern to another in response to the smallest of inputs. This changeability is a prime characteristic of many chaotic systems. In fact, we propose it is the very property that makes perception possible. We also speculate that chaos underlies the ability of the brain to respond flexibly to the outside world and to generate novel activity patterns, including those that are experienced as fresh ideas” [32].
Consistent with these remarks by Freeman, it has been demonstrated that trajectories through coherent states are indeed classical chaotic trajectories [17,37]. Therefore, even slight changes in the initial conditions may lead to diverging trajectories. For instance, in dreams or dream-like and sleeping states, where inputs are not so strong [8,27,38,39,40,41,42,43], the result is the occurrence of sudden shifts from one memory to another due to the chaoticity of the dynamics favored by its enhanced criticality. As a consequence, one may experience abruptly changing scenarios and feel overwhelmed by a series of emotions. These “debris” of memories might even be felt by the dreamer with the flavor of new, never-lived situations, as not belonging to his past, in that intricate blend or mix, presenting sometimes an obscure core, as the center of a vortex, which Freud has called [44] the “dream navel” [21].
It is also interesting that the common experience is that pain is absent in dreams, where the model predicts small correlated domains. On the other hand, it is known that the pain threshold may change under the effects of certain specific drugs, such as morphine, which may indeed be reducing the extent of neuronal connections.
In the case of anesthesia, after the patient resumes to the waking state, a sense of surprise that a period of time has passed since the effects of anesthesia commenced is frequently reported. This is clearly due to the patient’s detachment from the environment during the period under anesthesia. Anesthesiology studies appear to suggest that in the anesthetic recovery stages, certain dreaming or dream-like activity occurs [28,45].
It is interesting to ask whether the brain-mind visual experiences occur in certain comatose states as well, the mechanisms of which are not yet completely understood. Excluding the cases of extreme damage to the brain, closure to the external world leads to the conjecture that the brain-mind visual experiences may also occur in certain comatose states as well as in vegetative states, which are mediated by decreased coordinated activity among the small, short-lived, and unstable neuronal assemblies [46].
It should be noted, for the sake of completeness, that the formation of extended correlation domains may also be inhibited by a rapid succession of strong perceptual inputs which might dominate the emotional state of the subject; the subject’s arousal may reach extremely high levels, preventing him/her from focusing attention on any of the inputs. This may also occur when neuronal recruitment is enhanced by certain chemicals. Several competitive domains are formed in short time intervals, and their inflation would correspond to lack of information “in the average”. As a consequence, the subject is unable to respond to these inputs coherently, which translates to a deficiency in his/her functional relational activity [21].
In summary, in the afore-stated non-ordinary states, in the condition of quasi-closure, the subject’s response to the external noisy inputs is reduced considerably, and his/her level of awareness of the environmental changes is quite low or even absent in certain scenarios. Nevertheless, in such a state, the smallest of perturbations that are not filtered out may induce chaoticity in brain behavior. In the absence of reactive feedback, the subject becomes a “spectator of himself”, in a process of identification with his Double no longer distinguishable from himself, which occurs in the open, normal state of awareness.
4. Intentionality and Meanings, Truthfulness and Realism in Brain-Mind Visual Experiences
In the brain’s functional activity, one crucial element to be considered is the intentionality, which enters as a characteristic ingredient in action-perception cycle.
Pribram [5,47] stressed that there always exists a content of “attention" in perception and of “intention" in action. There is an active perception in one’s relation with the world, guided by one’s changeable volition and intention in pursuing one’s best to-be-in-the-world. Freeman remarked that neuronal activity serves “as a unified whole in shaping each intentional action at each moment” [48]. The brain constructs meanings out of perceptual experiences. Meanings arise from the dynamic correlations in the landscape of the attractors constructed out of the brain’s perceptual history [13,49,50,51,52], and are the basic substance of the subject’s identity, manifesting themselves in the “intended actions” following the “active perception.”
Such a profound intentional component emerges as “meaningfulness” in the brain-mind visual experiences as well. It might “guide” the chaotic trajectories leading to the “rearrangement” of memory traces into fresh scenarios and events which may even appear completely disjointed from the waking experience. Such traces are, however, related to the waking experience through the deep red thread of intentionality and meaningfulness, univocally associated to the subject’s identity, to the “affectivity [which] is the primordial form of subjectivity” [53]. This profound intentional component may represent the ‘unconscious wish’, postulated by Freud, in dreams [44], and has been recognized by Globus in the lucid dreaming phenomenon [54,55], where the dreamer has a certain form of control over the dream scenarios. This is why the brain-mind visual experiences carry relatively hidden, veiled meanings. According to the arguments presented in the previous section, visual scenarios are fed with “pieces” or “debris” of the previously recorded memories. Certain feelings or context settings might be anticipated in the brain-mind visual experiences on the basis of a persisting intentional component, and may then (re-)appear in the future perceptual experiences (cf. the narration in Appendix B). This is not to talk of any precognition capability or of violation of causation. It is merely to state that the correlations in the attractor landscape originating from the brain-mind visual experiences may, at certain times, find occasional resemblance to the correlations originating from the active perceptual experiences in a future waking state.
Therefore, in the movie-like flow of images in non-ordinary states, dreams, and other brain-mind visual experiences, the recollection of the existing correlations in the attractor landscape may indicate unforeseen contexts, giving rise to the problem of “truthfulness" or “realism" of such contexts and of their meanings as felt by the subject within the boundaries of his/her beliefs, knowledge, and emotional states.
In order to consider this point, it is observed that the algebra of the doubling of the degrees of freedom implies that $A_{\rm k}^{}$ and $\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ are entangled modes [37,56]. As a consequence, any ‘observation’ of the $A_{\rm k}^{}$ modes is, in fact, dependent on the $\widetilde{A}_{\rm k}^{}$ modes, which thus constitute the “address" for the $A_{\rm k}^{}$ modes, and vice-versa. The conclusion is that the brain modes and the mental (Double) modes cannot be separated, i.e., there exists no separation between mental activity and brain activity. This implies that a sort of “truth-evaluation-function" is implicit in the dissipative model formalism [7,57,58,59]. In other words, in the dialogue with one’s Double, the subject finds the possibility of confirming or rejecting the truthfulness of one’s working hypotheses. One’s “confidence" in one’s perceptual experiences is based on the process of feedback-adjustment-feedback, in the continuous matching with the Double. Intentional actions are planned in accordance with the hypotheses provided by the Double through the reconstruction of past perceptual experiences. The experience of changes in perception following repeated trials in the action creates the perception of time and causation [59,60].
It is stressed that each step of brain activity characterized by criticality is formally expressed by the free energy minimization condition. Recording a memory is the consequence of a process of breakdown of symmetry induced by the perceptive stimulus (cf. Appendix A). In one’s “active” selection among the perceptual inputs, one focuses “only on those inputs that one judges worthwhile to expend one’s energy for, the ones to which a “value” is attributed, those which involve one’s emotions [21], one’s affectivity” [53]. Using these selected inputs, one’s memory (non-oblivion) is constructed, which depicts one’s identity and one’s “truth”. Non-oblivion and truth coincide in the ancient Greek \alpha\lambda\eta\theta\epsilon\iota\alpha [21]. The landscape of the attractors constructed in the previous experiences is reshaped in this manner with any fresh input, and meanings are constructed. Memory is the memory of meanings [56,61].
All these features are present in the movie-like sequences in non-ordinary states, dreams, or dream-like states.
In brain-mind visual states, the perception of time is conditioned by the (quasi-)closure of the subject’s state. The lack of synchronization to a reference clock manifests in the loss of time-ordering of the events as recorded originally in the waking perceptual state. Mixing of memory traces with the emergence of fresh scenarios occurs then. The feelings of truthfulness and realism of these fresh scenarios derive from the fact that the traces of memories are actually traces of meanings, interwoven together by the red thread of intentionality stated earlier; they carry the seal of the subject’s identity.
Truthfulness and realism are further strengthened by the previously-stated matching, almost identification, of the self with its Double in the low level of openness. The (quasi-)closure of the subject and his/her emotional state are interrupted by the “waking-up” experience, thereby restoring the openness of the system and the awareness of the distinction between the self and the Double (It was only a dream!).
In the experience presented in Appendix B, the subject reported the “appearance of a gray painting, similar to the screen of an old black-and-white TV when the signal was lost; when that strange TV in the middle of the Amazon could “retune”, a new scene appeared”. Interestingly, this kind of “blacking-out of the signal” might find correspondence with the phenomenon of “null spike” (cf. Appendix A), which is observed to separate two behavioral frames in the brain activity [16]. Null spikes represent singularities appearing in the transitions between different dynamical regimes (phase transitions) corresponding to different configurations of correlations in the attractor landscape. The null spike behaves as a caesura, similar to the shutter diaphragm of old fashioned cameras. In the transition, the coherence of the condensate vanishes (null spike), and soon after, another coherent condensate initiates [62]. The phenomenon may be observed through an EEG in normal brain activity [18,63]. In brain-mind visual experiences, null spike corresponds to the closing of a movie-like flow, clearing the field, and opening of another movie-like flow.
5. Conclusion
Using the dissipative quantum model of the brain, the brain-mind visual experiences occurring during non-ordinary states, dreaming activity, meditation, and other low openness functional activities of the brain were discussed.
The low degree of openness of the brain in the afore-stated states induces enhancement of the criticality of the dynamics, which leads to enhancement in the possibility of “traveling” within the memory space through chaotic trajectories. Movie-like sequences of images and abrupt changes in the relatively familiar scenarios are generated consequently in the form of brain-mind visual experiences.
The feeling of truthfulness and realism associated with these visual experiences is derived from the quasi-identification of the self with the Double, a trace of the continual matching in the feedback-adjustment-feedback process occurring within the intentional context of the state of wakefulness.
It was conjectured that a similar description might also be applied to cases under anesthesia and certain pathological cases, such as certain states of coma and conditions where the openness of the brain toward the environment is reduced.
The existence of an infinite number of unitarily inequivalent representations of the canonical commutation relations in QFT, on which the space of the memory states is constructed, has been crucial to the analysis conducted in the present work.
According to the findings of the present work, a relatively severe closure of the brain to the world may produce a dramatic lack of “meaningfulness” within the subject’s action-perception cycle. Such conditions of closure occurring for certain reasons in the subjects during their wakefulness state might be the actual reason underlying the subject’s deficit in social communication and interaction, with the exhibition of restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities. It would be interesting to ask the question whether brain-mind visual experiences occur in such cases as well. The investigation of this query should be considered for future research, with the inclusion of autism spectrum disorder, if possible.
The present research work is dedicated to the memory of Karen Sharon and Eliano Pessa. Karen, during her association with the research work of Karl Pribram and Walter Freeman, contributed to the cultural atmosphere and dense activity at the foundation of modern neuroscience. Eliano, with his deep insights and expertise in neural networks and non-linear dynamical systems, provided important contributions to the formulation of the dissipative model.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Nicola Bragazzi, Gabriele Penazzi, Fabio Firenzuoli, Florencio Vicente, Bruno Neri and Riccardo Zerbetto for the fruitful discussions on the themes we presented in the article.
Additional Materials
Appendix A. The Dissipative Quantum Model of the Brain-a Summary.
Appendix B. The Narration of a Case of Brain-mind Visual Experiences Induced by Psychoactive substances.
Author Contributions
All authors have contributed equally to the work reported.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
References
- Prigogine I. Introduction to thermodynamics of irreversible processes. Springfield, Illinois, USA: Charles C. Thomas Publisher; 1955.
- Fröhlich H. Long‐range coherence and energy storage in biological systems. Int J Quantum Chem. 1968; 2: 641-649. [CrossRef]
- Ricciardi LM, Umezawa H. Brain and physics of many-body problems. Kybernetik. 1967; 4: 44-48. Reprint in Globus GG, Pribram KH, Vitiello G. Eds. Brain and being. At the boundary between science, philosophy, language and arts. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publ. Co.; 2004. (pp. 255-66).
- Lashley KS. The problem of cerebral organization in vision. Lancaster: Jacques Cattell Press; 1942
- Pribram KH. Brain and perception. Hillsdale, N.J., USA: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991.
- Freeman WJ. Mass action in the nervous system. New York, N.Y., USA: Academic Press; 1975.
- Vitiello G. Dissipation and memory capacity in the quantum brain model. Int J Mod Phys. 1995; 9: 973-989. [CrossRef]
- Schredl M. Abstracts of the 35th annual conference of the international association for the study of dreams June 16-June 20, 2018; Scottsdale, Arizona, USA. Int J Dream Res. 2018; 11: Supplement 1, S1-S69. https://DOI.org/10.11588/ijodr.2018.0.49912 [CrossRef]
- Fantegrossi WE, Murnane KS, Reissig CJ. The behavioral pharmacology of hallucinogens. Biochem Pharmacol. 2008; 75: 17-33. [CrossRef]
- Nichols DE. Psychedelics. Pharmacol Rev. 2016; 68: 264-355. [CrossRef]
- St John G. The breakthrough experience: DMT hyperspace and its liminal aesthetics. Anthr Consc. 2018; 29: 57-76. [CrossRef]
- Freeman W. Neurodynamics: An exploration in mesoscopic brain dynamics. Berlin: Springer; 2000 [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ, Vitiello G. Nonlinear brain dynamics as macroscopic manifestation of underlying many-body field dynamics. Phys Life Rev. 2006; 3: 93-118. [CrossRef]
- Gireesh ED, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches organize as nested theta-and beta/gamma-oscillations during development of cortical layer 2/3. Proc Natl Acad. 2008; 105: 7576-7581. [CrossRef]
- Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Neves CF. Consciousness as a phenomenon in the operational architectonics of brain organization: Criticality and self-organization considerations. Chaos Soliton Fract. 2013; 55: 13-31. [CrossRef]
- Freeman W, Quiroga RQ. Imaging brain function with EEG: Advanced temporal and spatial analysis of electroencephalographic signals. New York, N.Y. USA: Springer Science & Business Media; 2012.
- Vitiello G. Coherent states, fractals and brain waves. New Mat Nat Comput. 2009; 5: 245-264. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ. A cinematographic hypothesis of cortical dynamics in perception. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006; 60: 149-161. [CrossRef]
- Kozma R, Freeman WJ. Cognitive phase transitions in the cerebral cortex-enhancing the neuron doctrine by modeling neural fields. Switzerland: Springer Int. Pub.; 2016. [CrossRef]
- Kozma R, Davis JJ, Freeman WJ. Synchronized minima in ECoG power at frequencies between beta-gamma oscillations disclose cortical singularities in cognition. J Neurosci Neuroeng. 2012; 1: 13-23. [CrossRef]
- Vitiello G. My double unveiled. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publ. Co.; 2001. [CrossRef]
- Alfinito E, Vitiello G. Formation and life-time of memory domains in the dissipative quantum model of brain. Int J Mod Phys. 2000; 14: 853-868. [CrossRef]
- Greenfield SA. How might the brain generate consciousness? Communi Cogn. 1997; 30: 285-300.
- Vitiello G. The dissipative brain. In Globus GG, Pribram KH, Vitiello G. Eds. Brain and Being. At the boundary between science, philosophy, language and arts. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publ. Co.; 2004. (pp. 315-334).
- Horton CL. Consciousness across sleep and wake: Discontinuity and continuity of memory experiences as a reflection of consolidation processes. Front Psychiat. 2017; 8: 159. [CrossRef]
- Reinsel R, Antrobus JS, Wollman M. Bizarreness in dreams and waking fantasy. Hillsdale, N.J., USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 1992.
- Hobson JA. Dreaming: An introduction to the science of sleep. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2002.
- Leslie K, Skrzypek H, Paech MJ, Kurowski I, Whybrow T. Dreaming during anesthesia and anesthetic depth in elective surgery patients: A prospective cohort study. Anesthesiology. 2007; 106: 33-42. [CrossRef]
- Lutz A, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness. In Cambridge handbook of consciousness: Zelazo P, Moscovitch M, Thompson E, Eds. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2007.
- Josipovic Z. Duality and nonduality in meditation research. Conscious Cogn. 2010; 19: 1119-1121. [CrossRef]
- Zaccaro A, Piarulli A, Laurino M, Garbella E, Menicucci D, Neri B, et al. How breath-control can change your life: A systematic review on psycho-physiological correlates of slow breathing. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12: 353. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ. The physiology of perception. Sci Am. 1991; 264: 78-87. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ. Random activity at the microscopic neural level in cortex (“noise”) sustains and is regulated by low-dimensional dynamics of macroscopic cortical activity (“chaos”). Int J Neural Syst. 1996; 7: 473-480. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ. A field-theoretic approach to understanding scale-free neocortical dynamics. Biol Cybern. 2005; 92: 350-359. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ, O’ Nuillain S, Rodriguez J. Simulating cortical background activity at rest with filtered noise. J Integr Neurosci. 2008; 7: 337-344. [CrossRef]
- Tsuda I. Toward an interpretation of dynamic neural activity in terms of chaotic dynamical systems. Behav Brain Sci. 2001; 24: 793-810. [CrossRef]
- Pessa E, Vitiello G. Quantum noise, entanglement and chaos in the quantum field theory of mind/brain states. Mind Matter. 2003; 1: 59-79.
- Panksepp J. The dream of reason creates monsters...especially when we neglect the role of emotions in REM-states. Behav Brain Sci. 2000; 23: 988-990. [CrossRef]
- Louie K, Wilson MA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron. 2001; 29: 145-156. [CrossRef]
- Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron. 2002; 36: 1183-1194. [CrossRef]
- Pelayo R, Dement W. History of sleep physiology and medicine. In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement W. Eds. Principles and practice of sleep medicine, Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier; 2017 (pp.3-14). http://dx.DOI.org/10.1016/B978-0-323-24288-2.00001-5 [CrossRef]
- Ambrosini M, Giuditta A. Learning and sleep: The sequential hypothesis. Sleep Med Rev. 2001; 5: 477-490. [CrossRef]
- Shanor K, Kanwal J. Bats sing, mice giggle: The surprising science of animals’ inner lives. London, UK: Icon Books; 2009. (pp. 134-135).
- Freud S. [Reprinted 1965]. The interpretation of dreams. New York, N.Y., USA: Avon; 1900.
- Gyulaházi J, Redl P, Karányi Z, Varga K, Fülesdi B. Dreaming under anesthesia: Is it a real possiblity? Investigation of the effect of preoperative imagination on the quality of postoperative dream recalls. BMC Anesthesiol. 2015; 16: 53. [CrossRef]
- Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Bagnato S, Boccagni C, Galardi G. Toward operational architectonics of consciousness: Basic evidence from patients with severe cerebral injuries. Cogn Process. 2012; 13: 111-131. [CrossRef]
- Pribram KH. The form within: My point of view. Westport, CT, USA: Prospecta Press; 2013.
- Freeman WJ. Nonlinear neurodynamics of intentionality. J Mind behav. 1997; 18: 291-304.
- Freeman WJ, Gaál G, Jorsten R. A neurobiological theory of meaning in perception part III: Multiple cortical areas synchronize without loss of local autonomy. Int J Bifurc Chaos. 2003; 13: 2845-2856. [CrossRef]
- Freeman WJ, Rogers LJ. A neurobiological theory of meaning in perception part V: Multicortical patterns of phase modulation in gamma EEG. Int J Bifurc Chaos. 2003; 13: 2867-2887. [CrossRef]
- Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Neves CF. Natural world physical, brain operational, and mind phenomenal space-time. Phys Life Rev. 2010; 7: 195-249. [CrossRef]
- Vitiello G. The use of many-body physics and thermodynamics to describe the dynamics of rhythmic generators in sensory cortices engaged in memory and learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015; 31: 7-12. [CrossRef]
- Alcaro A, Carta S, Panksepp J. The affective core of the self: A neuro-archetypical perspective on the foundations of human (and animal) subjectivity. Front Psychol. 2017; 8: 1424. [CrossRef]
- Globus G. Lucid existenz during dreaming. Int J Dream Res. 2019; 12: 70-74.
- Globus G. Lucid dreaming and world creation: Ontological implications. Mind Matter. 2019; 17: 187-204.
- Sabbadini SA, Vitiello G. Entanglement and phase-mediated correlations in quantum field theory. Application to brain-mind states. App Sci. 2019; 9: 3203. DOI:10.3390/app9153203 [CrossRef]
- Basti G, Capolupo A, Vitiello G. Quantum field theory and coalgebraic logic in theoretical computer science. Prog Biophys Mol Biol. 2017; 130: 39-52. [CrossRef]
- Piattelli-Palmarini M, Vitiello G. Linguistics and some aspects of its underlying dynamics. Biolinguistics. 2015; 9: 96-115.
- Freeman WJ. Perception of time and causation through the kinesthesia of intentional action. Cogn Process. 2000; 1: 5-22.
- Freeman W, Vitiello G. Matter and mind are entangled in two streams of images guiding behavior and informing the subject through awareness. Mind Matter. 2016; 14: 7-24.
- Vitiello G. The brain and its mindful double. J Conscious Stu. 2018; 25: 151-176.
- Freeman WJ, Vitiello G. Vortices in brain waves. Int J Mod Phys. 2010; 24: 3269-3295. [CrossRef]
- Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA. Brain-mind Operational Architectonics imaging: Technical and methodological aspects. Open Neuroimag J. 2008; 2: 73-93. DOI: 10.2174/1874440000802010073 [CrossRef]
- Capolupo A, Kozma R, Olivares del Campo A, Vitiello G. Bessel-like functional distributions in brain average evoked potentials. J Integr Neurosci. 2017; 16: S85-S98. [CrossRef]
- Goldstone J, Salam A, Weinberg S. Broken symmetries. Phys Rev. 1962; 127: 965. [CrossRef]
- Itzykson C, Zuber JB. Quantum field theory. New York, N.Y., USA: McGraw-Hill; 1980.
- Umezawa H, Matsumoto H, Tachiki M. Thermo field dynamics and condensed states. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland; 1982.
- Umezawa H. Advanced field theory: Micro, macro, and thermal physics, New York, N.Y., USA: American Institute of Physics; 1993.
- Jibu M, Yasue K. Quantum brain dynamics and consciousness. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: J. Benjamins Pub. Co.; 1995. [CrossRef]
- Shulgin AT, Shulgin A. TIHKAL: The continuation. Berkeley, CA, USA.: Transform Press; 1997.
- Rudgley R. The encyclopedia of psychoactive substances. New York, N.Y., USA: Thomas Dunne; 2000.
- Orsolini L, Ciccarese M, Papanti D, De Berardis D, Guirguis A, Corkery JM, et al. Psychedelic fauna for psychonaut hunters: A mini-review. Front Psychiat. 2018; 9: 153. [CrossRef]
- Franzen F, Gross H. Tryptamine, N, N-dimethyltryptamine, N, N-dimethyl-5-hydroxytryptamine and 5-methoxytryptamine in human blood and urine. Nature. 1965; 206: 1052. DOI: 10.1038/2061052a0 [CrossRef]
- Saavedra JM, Axelrod J. Psychotomimetic N-methylated tryptamines: Formation in brain in vivo and in vitro. Science. 1972; 175: 1365-1366. [CrossRef]
- Jacob MS, Presti DE. Endogenous psychoactive tryptamines reconsidered: An anxiolytic role for dimethyltryptamine. Med Hypotheses. 2005; 64: 930-937. [CrossRef]
- Re T, Ventura C. Transcultural perspective on consciousness: A bridge between anthropology, medicine and physics. Cosm Hist. 2015; 11: 228-241.
- Presti DE. Foundational concepts in neuroscience. New York, N.Y., USA: W.W. Norton & Company; 2016.
- Bragazzi NL, Khabbache H, Vecchio I, Martini M, Perduca M, Zerbetto R, et al. Ancient shamanism and modern psychotherapy: From athropology to evidence-Based psychodelic medicine. Cosm Hist. 2018; 14: 142-152.
- Ruzo A. The boiling river. New York: Ted Books; 2016. ISBN-13: 978-1501119477
- Worrall S. This river kills everything that falls into it. National Geographic. 2016. www.nationalgeographic.com/news/2016/03/160313-boiling-river-amazon-geothermal-science-conservation-ngbooktalk/[accessed 10 September 2019]
- MacDonald F. Scientists found a mysterious ’boiling’ river straight out of amazonian legend. Science Alert. https://www.sciencealert.com/scientists-found-a-mysterious-boiling-river-straight-out-of-amazonian-legend [accessed 10 September 2019]