OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine is an international peer-reviewed Open Access journal published quarterly online by LIDSEN Publishing Inc. It covers all evidence-based scientific studies on integrative, alternative and complementary approaches to improving health and wellness.

Topics contain but are not limited to:

  • Acupuncture
  • Acupressure
  • Acupotomy
  • Bioelectromagnetics applications
  • Pharmacological and biological treatments including their efficacy and safety
  • Diet, nutrition and lifestyle changes
  • Herbal medicine
  • Homeopathy
  • Manual healing methods (e.g., massage, physical therapy)
  • Kinesiology
  • Mind/body interventions
  • Preventive medicine
  • Research in integrative medicine
  • Education in integrative medicine
  • Related policies

It publishes a variety of article types: Original Research, Review, Communication, Opinion, Comment, Conference Report, Technical Note, Book Review, etc.

There is no restriction on paper length, provided that the text is concise and comprehensive. Authors should present their results in as much detail as possible, as reviewers are encouraged to emphasize scientific rigor and reproducibility.

Indexing: DOAJ-Directory of Open Access Journals.

Publication Speed (median values for papers published in 2023): Submission to First Decision: 5.9 weeks; Submission to Acceptance: 14.7 weeks; Acceptance to Publication: 8 days (1-2 days of FREE language polishing included)

Open Access Review

Meditation, Sleep, and Performance

Lauren E. Guerriero , Bruce F. O’Hara *

Department of Biology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA

Correspondence: Bruce F. O’Hara

Academic Editors: Sok Cheon Pak and Soo Liang Ooi

Special Issue: Health Benefits of Meditation

Received: February 12, 2019 | Accepted: May 22, 2019 | Published: May 24, 2019

OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine 2019, Volume 4, Issue 2, doi:10.21926/obm.icm.1902031

Recommended citation: Guerriero LE, O’Hara BF. Meditation, Sleep, and Performance. OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine 2019; 4(2): 031; doi:10.21926/obm.icm.1902031.

© 2019 by the authors. This is an open access article distributed under the conditions of the Creative Commons by Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is correctly cited.

Abstract

Meditation describes a large variety of traditions that are extremely variable, but all include the conscious focus or awareness of attention. By maintaining their attention, meditators experience both acute and long-term changes in physiology, anatomy, and cognitive performance. The literature shows that the type of performance benefits may depend on the specific type of mental training. During meditation practice there is a documented increase in neuronal coordination and slowing of neuronal firing across many regions in the brain, a similar process to nonREM sleep physiology. Due to these commonalities, meditation may reduce the homeostatic pressures of sleep need and positively impact sleep architecture. Poor sleep and sleep loss are known to negatively affect performance, but meditation may be able to overcome these fatigue-induced detriments. Another factor that negatively affects performance is excessive stress, which is known to be impacted by meditation and sleep. The bidirectional relationship of meditation and sleep is apparent, but the mechanism is still unknown showing a need for more systematic investigations into the relationship between meditation, sleep, and improved performance. Meditation shares neurophysiological similarities with sleep, and these processes may have similar effects on improving attentional and cognitive performance. Also, limited evidence shows that performance detriments due to sleep loss may be partially overcome by meditation.

Keywords

Meditation; sleep; performance; stress; attention; cognition; physiology

1. Introduction

The conscious control of attention is the similarity of all meditation traditions, but meditation has been utilized for a variety reasons related to religion, self-actualization, stress reduction, and/or improving mental well-being. Meditation comes from a variety of cultures, with some of its earliest references in India going as far back as 3000 BCE. Since that time, almost all major religions have developed some form of meditation, showing the pervasive appeal of these practices.

Meditation research, at least in the “West”, became more popular in the 1960s and 70s when researchers began looking at the physiology of yogis, and practitioners of Transcendental meditation (TM) and mindfulness meditations in more detail. Yoga is wide variety of religious and philosophical traditions that span many centuries in India, with meditative practices focusing on inner peace or enlightenment achieved in part by reducing distractions due to internal or external stimuli. TM is a type of concentrative meditation that focuses on a mantra/sound which was brought to the West in the late 1950’s. Another widely studied type of meditation, mindfulness is a broad category of practices that focus on external and internal observations without passing judgement. Both mindfulness and yoga meditations were shown to induce a hypometabolic state, with practitioners having slower respiration rates, decreased galvanic skin response, and reduced heart rate [1,2,3]. TM and other concentrative meditations have also been shown to induce a hypometabolic state during practice, as compared to baseline conditions [4,5,6]. These changes have been attributed to lower sympathetic nervous system activation [7,8] and increases parasympathetic activation [9]. These findings are not conclusive with other studies showing meditation did not change or was even shown to increase heart rate or respiration [10]. The inconsistency of these results are likely attributed to the variability in meditation practices and experimental designs.

Studies of this era showed that experienced meditators had different neurophysiology as compared to non-meditators, with early studies using electroencephalography (EEG) to investigate the neuronal dynamics during meditation. Early EEG studies show the slowing of cortical neuronal firing as a result of meditation, resulting in the increased appearance of alpha and theta rhythms when awake, although the interpretation of this cortical slowing differed between studies [2,11,12,13,14,15]. Specifically, some authors mischaracterized that their EEG traces showed increases in alpha activity that were described as looking visually similar to early sleep [11,16,17]. When subjects were asked, or when their meditation was interrupted, the meditators reported being awake, showing that this visual similarity of the signal was misleading and misinterpreted [13]. Other EEG studies of this time specifically reported that the cortical activity was clearly distinct from sleep [18,19].

During this time, analyses of performance changes and cognition and their relation to meditation neurophysiology were not systematically studied. Few studies included some reaction time studies, stating that TM practitioners had faster reaction times than non-meditator groups [20] and TM practice increases reaction time from baseline [21]. Beyond this, there were occasional reports of exceptional abilities in long-term meditators. Anand, Chhina, and Singh [11] reported that a subset of their yogi subjects were more resistant to pain induced by putting one’s hand in cold water for 45-55 min. The pain response to temperature has been more recently studied in TM practitioners, showing that experienced meditators and also those with 5 months of practice had lessened brain activation to painful hot water [22]. Another study of the 1960s, Kasamatsu and Hirari [2], reported that some Zazen meditators in their study had increased far-sighted vision. Other mentions of performance enhancement were seen in that experienced meditators were found to be able to habituate more quickly to distracting and stressful stimuli, showing the anxiety reducing properties of meditation [11,22,23,24]. Meditation practice was also shown to acutely reduce levels of anxiety [23,25]. Although far from systematic, meditation research did have these occasional mentions of potential performance enhancement.

Meditation research has gained popularity since this time, but there is still much unknown in this field. The clinical benefits of meditation have been shown in a variety of clinical studies, to improve insomnia, ADHD, anxiety disorders, and hypertension [26,27,28,29,30,31]. There have even been claims that meditation elongates life [32]. Even though the benefits of meditation on disease states have been shown, there is still very limited data and a general lack of knowledge of the processes involved in meditation and how they interact with normal or abnormal physiology. Normal attention and cognitive performance can be impacted by sleep quality and quantity, meditation experience, and stress. This review will summarize the literature to date on how meditation’s neurophysiological changes impact attentional and cognitive performance, as well how meditation and performance interact with sleep.

2. Temporal Aspects of Meditation and Performance

Broadly, meditation traditions can be split into two broad categories based on the object of focus: focused attention and open monitoring. Concentrative or focused attention (FA) meditations rely on focus on a single “object” (such as the breath, body part, a single word, or a mantra). One type of FA is transcendental meditation (TM), which specifically focuses on a mantra or sound. The other category is, in some sense, the opposite with the focus of open monitoring (OM) or mindfulness meditations not being on any single stimulus or experience, but having the intention is to not pass judgment on any stimulus or thought. More recent categorization of meditation also includes loving kindness meditation as a third distinct type of meditation [33]. Compassion or loving kindness (LK) meditations have elements of both FA and OM meditations, but with the focus on developing love and kindness toward the self and then extending to others [33]. Current research broadly considers each different type of meditation to induce specific neuro-physiological changes. These specifics of each meditation type are outside the scope of this paper and have been reviewed by others [34,35,36].

Regardless of the type of meditation, the changes induced by meditation can be broken into two categories based on timing. State changes are those short-term changes that take place directly during or after meditation practice. Trait changes, on the other hand, are more permanent changes that take place after extensive meditation practice and repetition of the same attentional processes. These are thoroughly reviewed by Cahn and Polich [34].

2.1 States

When considering the timing of meditation’s effects, those taking place during meditation or immediately after are classified as state changes. Due to the lack of long term changes due to meditation, the neurological state changes that directly result from meditation practice can most easily be studied in novice meditators. Without this experience and long term exposure to meditation, there are no trait changes to influence the brain and performance. Most state effects of meditation are directly due to the variable neuronal activity that takes place during the session. Studies using EEG and fMRI support the notion that each type of meditation has some characteristic neuronal dynamics; while there are also some similarities, each meditation type has a different focus resulting in differential neuronal activation. This literature has been reviewed previously [34,35,36].

One widely reported state change indicative of meditation practice are EEG alpha (8-13 Hz) power increases and increases in alpha synchrony. This indicates the slowing and increasing coordination of cortical neurons during meditation sessions. Alpha power increase are commonly seen due to simply closing eyes in the occipital lobe [37,38,39], but this localization during meditation is in the frontal and parietal lobes with distinct regionalization based on meditation type [12,35,40,41,42]. The amount of coordination may rely on the amount of meditation experience with experienced meditators having higher alpha power than novices [2,36,43,44]. This increase in alpha power has been associated with increased relaxation and attention that takes place during meditation [34,45] and can even be induced from simple paced breathing [46,47]. This increased prevalence of alpha frequency represents a slowing and coordination of neuronal firing throughout the cerebral cortex.

This slowing of neuronal firing can even extend into the theta frequency range (4-8 Hz). The physiological change commonly attributed to FA meditation is increase in EEG theta power [12,36,46,48]. This neuroelectrical activity is similar to that which takes place during attention tasks [49], complex cognitive tasks [50], and during consciousness and sensory perception [51]. Also during FA meditation, there is an increase in theta synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association regions during meditation, showing the coordination of these regions [45,52], which are associated with working memory [53,54] and learning [55]. It is possible that this coordination of neuronal activity is improving performance after meditation due to the similar neurophysiological activity between meditation and the task being tested [34].

FA meditations have been shown to increase performance after short training. Due to FA utilizing attentional processes on a specific object of focus, attentional tasks may be more susceptible to improvements from FA than other types of meditation. Three weeks of training in FA (Dhammakaya Buddhist) meditation showed a faster reaction time [56]. This short amount of training may not be necessary for improved performance; reaction time and psychomotor vigilance have also been shown to improve after a single FA meditation session [57].

Meditation has been shown to mobilize mental resources and improve information processing. Attentional blink is a term that describes a deficit in identifying the second of two quickly presented stimuli, and meditation can lessen this via theta phase synchrony [58]. The mobilization of mental resources has been described in the literature to happen specifically after FA meditations and not after other meditation subtypes [59]. These other types of meditations have been shown to have performance benefits based on the focus being maintained during meditation. For example, OM practices are based on a broader attentional scope. Long term OM meditators have been shown to outperform FA meditators when there was an unexpected stimulus [60]. OM meditations, although they still involve conscious control of attention, have broader attention focuses than FA meditation, which could cause the ability to respond more quickly to unexpected stimuli.

Due to the specifics of each meditation, different types may be limited in their ability to improve task performance. But, meditation can benefit practitioners in other ways. Two studies utilized an 8-week Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction course (MBSR) and showed that subjects had no differences in attention before and after their training [61,62]. Even though attention was not affected, improvements in amounts of mindfulness and emotional well-being were reported after meditation [61].

OM may not affect attentional performance of normal, well-rested subjects, unlike FA traditions, but OM may be able to improve attention after sleep deprivation [63]. Losing sleep decreases attention and increases fatigue and sleepiness [63]. Rather than directly improving attention regulation and the associated regions in the brain, Kohler and colleagues stated that mindfulness can increase mental resource mobilization to counteract the increased sleep drive from lack of sleep [63]. OM meditations have been shown to increase occipital gamma power and decrease in delta power [64]. This bilateral, frontal decrease in delta power may reflect OM, decreasing the homeostatic pressure of sleep.

2.2 Traits

Research with long-term meditators brings into question the trait changes from their practice, and how these changes relate to task performance. One study of experienced Buddhist OM practitioners analysed their ability to supress interfering information using the Stroop task and d2-test of attention. During the Stroop task, subjects must say the color of the text rather than the word, and the d2-task that has participants cross out the letter “d” with two marks above or below it. These long-term practitioners showed that they had less Stroop interference than non-meditators, indicating better cognitive control and control of automatic responses [65]. Another study shows that this improved performance on the Stroop task demonstrates higher executive control and better emotional control in meditators of a variety of traditions [66]. Experienced meditators also performed better on the d2- test, having less errors of omission and commission, which indicate better attentional and inhibitory control, and better speed and accuracy of performance [65].

Hodgins and Adair used a large sample of meditators from different traditions [67]. When compared to non-meditators, the meditators showed less change blindness and were able to identify changes more quickly. They were also able to think more flexibly when describing an ambiguous image and were more flexibly able to redirect attention to new information. All of these data imply better visual perception and flexibility in processing visual information [67]. These performance improvements can be considered trait effects, because the subjects did not systematically meditate before testing, meaning performance enhancement is not due to the immediate effects of mediation.

But are these performance enhancements directly related to meditation experience? A study of experienced OM Zen meditators showed a correlation between amount of meditation practice and attention, mindfulness, and awareness [68]. As practitioners become more experienced and have more extreme trait changes, attentional focus and mindfulness may improve.

These improvements in performance may be due to the functional and anatomical changes associated with long-term meditation [69,70]. One physiological trait positively correlated with meditation practice is increased EEG gamma power. Studies have found that increased EEG gamma power in the parietal-occipital area takes place in experienced meditators during meditation but also during rest [71,72]. FA, OM, and a combined FA/OM meditations have all been shown to have higher mean gamma power both during meditation and an instructed mind-wandering task [72]. Since this happens both during meditation and persists afterward, this gamma power increase is most likely a trait change.

There have also been some reported anatomical changes due to long-term meditation practice. Expert meditators have also been found to have changes in cortical thickness in the prefrontal, frontal, and temporal cortices [73,74]. These regions are associated with attention, interoception, and sensory processing, which implies more cognitive function in these processes [73,74,75,76]. Meditation also leads to improved coherence between cortical regions, especially in the prefrontal and frontal regions [34,39].

The impact on drowsiness and fatigue may also be a trait of meditation. More years of daily OM meditation have been correlated with less drowsiness during a non-meditative thinking task [64]. This same study showed a correlation between number of daily meditation hours and drowsiness during meditation [64]. Meditation experience may impact how a person experiences lack of sleep and general fatigue. To understand how sleepiness and fatigue are impacted by meditation, the interactions between sleep and meditation must also be understood.

3. Meditation Experience and Sleep

Meditation and its impact on sleep was studied by Mason and colleagues [77] in TM. It was found that experienced TM meditators had greater REM density and less muscular activity during deep nREM sleep, as compared to short-training meditation controls [77]. This sample of experienced practitioners were also shown to have more stage 1 sleep, which is indicative of worse sleep. The implications of this study is that meditation can in fact impact sleep, although from this single study it was unclear if their TM meditation improved or worsened sleep.

More recent studies support this idea that experienced meditators may sleep differently than non-meditators. Total sleep time may be reduced in meditators, but this finding is based on only a few studies with small sample sizes [57,71]. Kaul and colleagues used actigraphy and self-report to determine that their subset of concentrative meditators who meditated over 2hrs per day slept only 5.2 hours as compared to the 7.8 hours of the non-meditating controls [57]. This effect of long-term concentrative meditation on sleep amount was also reported by Ferrarelli and colleagues, with experienced Buddhist meditators sleeping an average of 6.14 hours, as compared to meditation novices which slept an average of 6.75 hours [71]. In contrast, research in mindfulness and compassion meditations indicate no differences in total sleep time between long-term meditators and non-meditator controls [65,78,79,80]. It’s unclear if this is due to differences in meditation practices, as there have been no systematic studies of sleep duration and meditation experience. These findings raise the question: how does meditation influence sleep? Is this a trait of meditation experience or a state effect?

3.1 How Can Meditation Impact Sleep?

To determine if sleep is different between meditators and non-meditators, investigations must be made into the processes that control sleep, either via circadian influences or homeostatic drive. Meditation may impact melatonin secretion. One study of experienced TM practitioners, showed that meditation at night can increase melatonin, as compared to a night when they did not meditate [81]. The subjects in this study meditated during an unusual time, and more research needs to be completed with meditation during times of normal practice. Another study has shown that 3 months of yoga and FA meditation training can increase the amplitude of the night-time melatonin peak [82]. It is unclear by what mechanism meditation can impact melatonin dynamics. The impact of meditation on other circadian rhythms such as body temperature and sleep timing has yet to been studied in a comprehensive manner.

It is possible that meditation interacts with sleep via homeostatic processes. The most reliable measure of sleep need is slow wave activity (SWA) during the first bout of nREM sleep. SWA during nREM sleep can be predicted almost entirely by the amount of time spent awake, and this SWA decreases rapidly during nREM sleep [83]. Limited research indicates that meditation experience may have no effect on delta power during sleep [77,80], but no one has assessed acute effects of recent meditation on this process. If meditation works to decrease the homeostatic pressure of sleep, this may take place in faster frequencies (theta or alpha), as homeostatically controlled frequencies have been shown to extend from 0.25 to 12 Hz [83], well beyond the widely accepted delta band (0.25 to 4 Hz).

Investigations into sleep homeostasis and meditation show that long term yogic meditators have an increase in EEG lower frequency activity during nREM [80]. This study showed that the amount of increased low frequency activity depended on the amount of sleep. During the first episode of nREM sleep, meditators were found to have an increase in the EEG theta/alpha range of the prefrontal and parietal electrodes. But later in the night, during the second nREM episode and after some sleep debt has been “paid off”, this increase in low frequency wave shifts more into the delta slow wave frequency [80]. The first increase in the theta/alpha range may mimic the activity seen during meditation practice. By the third episode of nREM, there was no difference in low frequency activity between meditators and non-meditators [80]. Each sleep episode pays off more sleep debt, and the effects of meditation may dissipate as there is less homeostatic pressure. These studies may be difficult to interpret, as there are likely multiple and potentially opposing forces. For example, the neuronal synchronization seen in meditation may “pay-off” a small portion of sleep debt, thus reducing subsequent EEG delta power during initial sleep periods, but meditation may also reduce stress and improve sleep quality thus leading potentially to increased EEG delta power. Whether frequencies higher than delta (theta/alpha range) are relevant to paying off sleep debt is certainly not clear. Careful studies correlating specific frequency bands over specific cortical regions during meditation and during subsequent sleep may be able to parse these different processes.

Vipassana meditators and Kriya yogis have been shown to have increased amount of REM sleep [78,79]. This fits into the meditation- homeostasis framework, causing a lesser need for nREM sleep and allowing them to spend more time in REM sleep. These daily meditators did not have their meditation practice temporally controlled around the sleep study [78,79], and this raises questions on whether their finding was a state or trait effect of meditation on sleep. OM meditations have been shown to have differences in sleep architecture, although sleep time did not differ between meditator and non-meditator groups [78,79,80]. These studies also broke up their subjects into different age groups [78,79]. Middle aged Vipassana meditators (31-55 years) had enhanced amounts of REM sleep as compared to age matched controls [78]. These meditators also didn’t show the reduced amount of SWS seen in middle aged controls [78]. These effects seem to continue with more advanced aging, with a follow up study showing that older [50-60 years old] Vipassana meditators also had longer SWS duration and longer REM duration than age matched controls [79].

Sleep undergoes organizational changes and qualitative declines while aging. Reports show that older individuals are likely to have decreases in amount of SWS and REM sleep, and have increases in stage 1 sleep, wake after sleep onset, and sleep fragmentation [84,85,86]. Aging is also associated with lesser amplitude of delta power in SWS and lessened amplitudes of circadian rhythms [87,88]. The causes of these sleep phenomena over the lifespan are poorly understood. This may partially be due to the increased prevalence of sleep disorders such as insomnia, restless leg syndrome, nocturia, and other ailments [89,90,91]. Meditation has been found effective in reducing the severity of insomnia and restless leg syndrome [92,93,94]. If meditation is also able to protect against changes in sleep architecture and sleep duration, then meditation should be investigated to see if it prevents the cognitive decline seen with decreased sleep amounts [95]. Sleep issues are starting to be shown to be predictors of cognitive decline [86] and other disorders. There is a need for more longitudinal studies of experienced meditators and how their sleep and health changes over their lifespan, with special considerations when they are elderly.

Our current knowledge on meditation’s effect on sleep is skewed based on the few types of meditation that have been studied thus far. Detailed sleep of long-term Theravada or Tibetan Buddhism meditators has been directly investigated [71]. These traditions include components of both OM and FA (with focus on the breath) [71]. It was found that gamma power (25-42 Hz) during nREM sleep is strongest in the frontal and prefrontal areas and weakest in temporal [71]. In experienced meditators there was significantly higher gamma activity in parietal-occipital regions than naïve meditators. This did not take place during the first episode of nREM, but during the later three nREM episodes across their night. This nREM gamma power is correlated to amount of daily meditation practice [71]. Higher gamma during sleep could be reflecting that long term meditators are able to still maintain some activation in the sensory regions and Default Mode Network allowing them to maintain some level of awareness during sleep [71].

This potential increase of awareness during sleep has yet to be tested directly. Gamma power increases have also been recorded in the third nREM episode of LK and FA, which may indicate the traits of meditation in later sleep [80]. Gamma power increases may not be unique to meditators, as a subset of novices show this, but Lutz and colleagues stated that all experienced LK meditators in their study had the same increase in gamma power [96]. This may also be a result of this LK meditation as compared to the changes seen in FA. The literature on gamma power is growing, since historically gamma power has been filtered from different meditation EEG studies, but more recent analysis methods have allowed researchers to be able to distinguish gamma activity from muscular artifacts and other EEG noise.

EEG theta and alpha frequency oscillations have also been linked to maintenance of transcendental consciousness or awareness during sleep in TM practitioners, a process called witnessing sleep [77,97]. These experienced TM practitioners showed greater theta2 (6-8 Hz)–alpha1 (8-10 Hz) relative power during nREM sleep [77]. This amount of power shows a graded effect related to meditation experience, showing a trait of meditation in sleep [77]. More recently, Dentico and colleagues reported the same greater theta and alpha power in experienced meditators of both mindfulness and loving-compassion traditions [80]. The increase in theta and alpha power was visible after 8 hours of meditation during nREM sleep and was considered to be state changes as a “reactivation of the neuronal activity” during meditation [80]. This brings up the question if concentrative traditions may interplay with sleep differently than mindfulness or compassion based meditations. Reported results thus far may be skewed due to the few studies done on sleep, meditation, and performance.

3.2 Meditation and Sleep Loss

Reaction time and attention tasks are very susceptible to improvements via meditation. But after sleep loss, reaction time decreases due to fatigue and decreased maintenance of attention. The PVT, psychomotor vigilance task, measures reaction time and sustained attention in response to a visual stimulus [98]. Sleep restriction to 5 hours of sleep significantly worsens PVT performance and PVT lapses [63]. Those who underwent a 21-day meditation training had a performance boost after a session of meditation, which resulted in reaction times similar to pre-sleep restriction values, and a reduced number of lapses. The non-meditation controls did not get a performance boost nor a reduction in lapses [63]. Self-reported sleepiness after the sleep restrictions was reduced due to meditation, but not due to the control condition [63]. Self-reported fatigue or having low energy was also measured and subjects had no difference in amount of fatigue after meditation or their controls [63].

This same performance enhancing effect have been noted even after a whole night of sleep deprivation. Novice meditators, with no prior meditation training, underwent a whole night of sleep deprivation. The sleep deprivation significantly worsened PVT reaction time, but a single session of FA meditation was able to return reaction times close to baseline values [57]. Because of this strong effect in novices, meditation’s state changes can overcome some of the deficits due to sleep loss.

Due to the limited number of studies that looked at sleep deprivation in meditators, limited conclusions can be drawn. Meditation may be able to return PVT performance close to non-sleep deprived levels, even in novices. Other performance measures need to be tested. There are no data on how expert meditators respond to sleep deprivation. Also data need to be gathered on the neuronal dynamics of these processes after sleep deprivation or restriction.

4. Interactions with Stress

Although anxiety and cognitive performance are not believed to be directly correlated [99], stress can impact performance and memory [100]. Stress’ impact on performance has conflicting literature and multiple hypotheses have been posited on this topic [101].

Those with high anxiety are more prone to distractions and require more effort to complete a task [102,103]. Those with higher anxiety have been found to have longer reaction times, but comparable accuracy relative to those with low anxiety [100]. The attentional control theory states that stress may impair attentional control by shifting mental resources from the task at hand and reduces the influence of the goal-directed attentional system [100]. Meditation experience lowers basal levels of anxiety [104,105,106] and decreases the response to stress [107,108,109]. Meditation also has been shown to activate areas involved in the relaxation response [110]. If proven, the attention control theory would fit into the meditation and performance paradigm well, with meditation reducing stress and allowing more attentional resources to be utilized on task performance, but more research is needed.

Anxiety and sleep have a bidirectional relationship. Stress levels can directly impact sleep, and the amount and quality of sleep can impact anxiety. András and colleagues [111] found that higher levels of anxiety can increase sleep latency, reduce amounts of slow wave sleep, and decrease the amount of REM sleep. Since anxiety negatively impacts sleep, meditation may reduce anxiety therefore improving sleep and performance. Even one meditation session can increase the amount of mindfulness and positively affected mood [58,112]. The next step is for research to determine if this stress reduction caused by meditation can positively impact sleep as well.

Fragmented circadian rhythms have been associated with increased anxiety disorders in middle aged and elderly people [113]. As stated earlier, there is a lack of studies on meditation and circadian rhythms, so further circadian meditation research should also consider anxiety as part of that equation. At this point, we can say there appears to be multiple interactions between stress, meditation, sleep and performance. Future work in these areas should investigate causal interactions more directly.

5. Limits of Meditation and Future Directions

This literature review details the relationship between meditation, sleep, and performance, but most of these findings are based on very few papers that have limited conclusions. There is much work that needs to be done to conclusively describe these relationships between meditation, sleep, and performance.

Meditation can improve attention and emotional wellbeing, but has limitations on its enhancements, and a dedicated “fan” base that may exaggerate the benefits, or have a vested interest in positive results. The specific focus of meditation trains the associated regions in the brain, and may not help processes taking place primarily in unrelated regions. For example, the ability to improve cardiac interoception (the conscious perception of heart rate) was found to not be associated with meditation training, although regions associated with interoception are found to be increased during meditation [61,114,115]. Since cardiac interoception tasks are different than what is practiced during meditation, this may explain the negative results seen. Kohler and colleagues showed no difference in the Go/No-Go task between meditation and control activity groups [63]. These subjects practiced FA, Nidra yoga, meditation which is distinct from the response inhibition to a stimulus that is measured with the Go/No-Go task [116]. These results are heavily based on the type of meditation, as other meditation practices may impact cardiac interoception or the Go/No-Go task. The effects of meditation may be very specific and do not improve all performance ubiquitously.

Most of the studies included in this review use different types of meditation. Due to the different neuronal aspects of each meditation subtype, conclusions drawn here are far from conclusive on how meditation interacts with sleep and on subsequent performance. There is a need for more systematic study of sleep from those who practice different meditation techniques. Also, there needs to be more studies that directly compare different meditation types using multiple panels of cognitive tasks.

It is also unknown how long a single meditation session can impact the meditator. No current study has charted out the improvement in performance over time. Because the state effect of meditation is of unknown duration, the trait effects of meditation are difficult to separate from other influences. Beyond that, trait effects are also difficult to study, partly due to the difficulty of finding a comparable control group to long-term meditators. There may be certain traits that predispose someone to successful meditation and getting greater benefits from said meditation [117]. There are also many different covariables that may vary drastically between long-term meditators and non-meditators, such as diet, activity level, drug use, amount of motivation, and the ability to master a task. It is impossible to determine if this is directly due to the meditation training, or to other differences between groups. Performance studies need to be done on subjects with meditation experience, but without recent meditation practice. This would allow for traits to be clearly distinguished from state effects.

As this paper demonstrates sleep, meditation experience, and stress can all impact performance. Many of these studies have found correlations between these variables, but the causal relationship is unclear. The ability to meditate effectively and the subsequent performance boost may depend on stress levels, a previous night’s sleep, circadian factors, or other variables. Research on meditation is growing, but still has a long way to go.

Acknowledgments

We thank Shreyas Joshi for helping with compiling our sources.

Author Contributions

Both L.E. Guerriero and B.F. O’Hara contributed to the writing and conceptual ideas included in this manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

References

  1. Bagchi BKW, M.A. Simultaneous EEG and other recordings during some yogic practices. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1958; 193.
  2. Kasamatsu A, Hirai T. An electroencephalographic study on the zen meditation (Zazen). Folia Psychiatr Neurol Jpn. 1966; 20: 315-336. [CrossRef]
  3. Wallace RK. Physiological effects of transcendental meditation. Science. 1970; 167: 1751-1754. [CrossRef]
  4. Benson H, Steinert RF, Greenwood MM, Klemchuk HM, Peterson NH. Continuous measurement of O2 consumption and CO2 elimination during a wakeful hypometabolic state. J Hum Stress. 1975; 1: 37-44. [CrossRef]
  5. Elson BD, Hauri P, Cunis D. Physiological changes in yoga meditation. Psychophysiology. 1977; 14: 52-57. [CrossRef]
  6. Morse DR, Martin JS, Furst ML, Dubin LL. A physiological and subjective evaluation of meditation, hypnosis, and relaxation. Psychosom Med. 1977; 39: 304-324. [CrossRef]
  7. Davidson JM. The physiology of meditation and mystical states of consciousness. Perspect Biol Med. 1976; 19: 345-379. [CrossRef]
  8. Lang R, Dehof K, Meurer KA, Kaufmann W. Sympathetic activity and transcendental meditation. J Neural Transm. 1979; 44: 117-135. [CrossRef]
  9. Bujatti M, Biederer P. Serotonin, noradrenaline, dopamine metabolites in transcendental meditation-technique. J Neural Transm. 1976; 39: 257-267. [CrossRef]
  10. Das NNG, H. Variations de l'activite electrique du cerveau du coeur et des muscles quelletiques au cours de la meditation et de l'extase yogique. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1957; 6: 211-219.
  11. Anand BKC, G.S.; Singh, B. Some aspects of electroencephalograhic studies in Yogis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1961; 13: 452-456. [CrossRef]
  12. Banquet JP. Spectral analysis of the EEG in meditation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1973; 35: 143-151. [CrossRef]
  13. Hebert R, Lehmann D. Theta bursts: an EEG pattern in normal subjects practising the transcendental meditation technique. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1977; 42: 397-405. [CrossRef]
  14. Ghista DN, Nandagopal D, Ramamurthi B, Das A, Mukherji A, Srinivasan T. Physiological characterisation of the ‘meditative state’during intuitional practice (the Ananda Marga system of mediation) and its therapeutic value. Med Biol Eng. 1976; 14: 209-214. [CrossRef]
  15. Delmonte M. Electrocoritcal activity and related phenomena associated with meditation practice: A literature review. Int J Neurosci. 1984; 24: 217-231. [CrossRef]
  16. Younger J, Adriance W, Berger RJ. Sleep during transcendental meditation. Percept Mot Skills. 1975; 40: 953-954. [CrossRef]
  17. Pagano RR, Rose RM, Stivers RM, Warrenburg S. Sleep during transcendental meditation. Science. 1976; 191: 308-310. [CrossRef]
  18. Corby JC, Roth WT, Zarcone VP, Kopell BS. Psychophysiological correlates of the practice of Tantric Yoga meditation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978; 35: 571-577. [CrossRef]
  19. Barwood T, Empson J, Lister S, Tilley A. Auditory evoked potentials and transcendental meditation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1978; 45: 671-673. [CrossRef]
  20. Appelle S, Oswald LE. Simple reaction time as a function of alertness and prior mental activity. Percept Mot Skills. 1974; 38: 1263-1268. [CrossRef]
  21. Holt WR, Caruso JL, Riley JB. Transcendental meditation vs pseudo-meditation on visual choice reaction time. Percept Mot Skills. 1978; 46: 726. [CrossRef]
  22. Orme-Johnson DW, Schneider RH, Son YD, Nidich S, Cho ZH. Neuroimaging of meditation's effect on brain reactivity to pain. Neuroreport. 2006; 17: 1359-1363. [CrossRef]
  23. Puryear HB, Cayce CT, Thurston MA. Anxiety reduction associated with meditation: home study. Percept Mot Skills. 1976; 42: 527-531. [CrossRef]
  24. Goleman D, Schwartz G. Meditation as an intervention in stress reactivity. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1976; 44: 456. [CrossRef]
  25. Wallace RK, Benson H. The physiology of meditation. Scientific American. 1972; 226: 84-90. [CrossRef]
  26. Janssen L, Kan CC, Carpentier PJ, Sizoo B, Hepark S, Grutters J, et al. Mindfulness based cognitive therapy versus treatment as usual in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). BMC psychiatry. 2015; 15: 216. [CrossRef]
  27. Zylowska L, Ackerman DL, Yang MH, Futrell JL, Horton NL, Hale TS, et al. Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: a feasibility study. J Atten Disord. 2008; 11: 737-746. [CrossRef]
  28. Miller JJ, Fletcher K, Kabat-Zinn J. Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1995; 17: 192-200. [CrossRef]
  29. Mitchell JT, McIntyre EM, English JS, Dennis MF, Beckham JC, Kollins SH. A pilot trial of mindfulness meditation training for ADHD in adulthood: impact on core symptoms, executive functioning, and emotion dysregulation. J Atten Disord. 2017; 21: 1105-1120. [CrossRef]
  30. King MS, Carr T, D'Cruz C. Transcendental meditation, hypertension and heart disease. Aust Fam Physician. 2002; 31: 164.
  31. Walton KG, Pugh ND, Gelderloos P, Macrae P. Stress reduction and preventing hypertension: preliminary support for a psychoneuroendocrine mechanism. J Altern Complement Med. 1995; 1: 263-283. [CrossRef]
  32. Alexander CN, Langer EJ, Newman RI, Chandler HM, Davies JL. Transcendental meditation, mindfulness, and longevity: an experimental study with the elderly. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989; 57: 950. [CrossRef]
  33. Lippelt DP, Hommel B, Colzato LS. Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity–A review. Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 1083. [CrossRef]
  34. Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132: 180-211. [CrossRef]
  35. Kaur C, Singh P. EEG derived neuronal dynamics during meditation: Progress and challenges. Adv Prev Med. 2015; 2015: 614723. [CrossRef]
  36. Lee DJ, Kulubya E, Goldin P, Goodarzi A, Girgis F. Review of the neural oscillations underlying meditation. Front Neurosci. 2018; 12: 178. [CrossRef]
  37. Schürmann M, Başar E. Functional aspects of alpha oscillations in the EEG. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001; 39: 151-158. [CrossRef]
  38. Cantero JL, Atienza M, Salas RM. Human alpha oscillations in wakefulness, drowsiness period, and REM sleep: different electroencephalographic phenomena within the alpha band. Neurophysiol Clin. 2002; 32: 54-71. [CrossRef]
  39. Jian-Zhou Z, Jing-Zhen L, Qing-Nian H. Statistical brain topographic mapping analysis for EEGs recorded during Qi Gong state. Int J Neurosci. 1988; 38: 415-425. [CrossRef]
  40. Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Schimke H, Pachinger T. Alpha frequency, reaction time, and the speed of processing information. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1996; 13: 511-518. [CrossRef]
  41. Dunn BR, Hartigan JA, Mikulas WL. Concentration and mindfulness meditations: unique forms of consciousness? Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 1999; 24: 147-165. [CrossRef]
  42. Lomas T, Ivtzan I, Fu CH. A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 57: 401-410. [CrossRef]
  43. Takahashi T, Murata T, Hamada T, Omori M, Kosaka H, Kikuchi M, et al. Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005; 55: 199-207. [CrossRef]
  44. Arambula P, Peper E, Kawakami M, Gibney KH. The physiological correlates of Kundalini Yoga meditation: a study of a yoga master. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2001; 26 : 147-153. [CrossRef]
  45. Aftanas L, Golocheikine S. Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: high-resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neurosci Lett. 2001; 310: 57-60. [CrossRef]
  46. Stancák JA, Pfeffer D, Hrudfová L, Sovka P, Dostálek C. Electroencephalographic correlates of paced breathing. Neuroreport. 1993; 4: 723-726. [CrossRef]
  47. Fumoto M, Sato-Suzuki I, Seki Y, Mohri Y, Arita H. Appearance of high-frequency alpha band with disappearance of low-frequency alpha band in EEG is produced during voluntary abdominal breathing in an eyes-closed condition. Neurosci Res. 2004; 50: 307-317. [CrossRef]
  48. Mizuki Y, Tanaka M, Isozaki H, Nishijima H, Inanaga K. Periodic appearance of theta rhythm in the frontal midline area during performance of a mental task. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1980; 49: 345-351. [CrossRef]
  49. Kubota Y, Sato W, Toichi M, Murai T, Okada T, Hayashi A, et al. Frontal midline theta rhythm is correlated with cardiac autonomic activities during the performance of an attention demanding meditation procedure. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001; 11: 281-287. [CrossRef]
  50. Mizuhara H, Wang L-Q, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y. A long-range cortical network emerging with theta oscillation in a mental task. Neuroreport. 2004;15:1233-1238. [CrossRef]
  51. Kjaer TW, Bertelsen C, Piccini P, Brooks D, Alving J, Lou HC. Increased dopamine tone during meditation-induced change of consciousness. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2002; 13: 255-259. [CrossRef]
  52. Aftanas LI, Golosheikin SA. Changes in cortical activity in altered states of consciousness: the study of meditation by high-resolution EEG. Hum Physiol. 2003; 29: 143-151. [CrossRef]
  53. Sarnthein J, Petsche H, Rappelsberger P, Shaw G, Von Stein A. Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. PNAS. 1998; 95: 7092-7096. [CrossRef]
  54. Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Rev. 1999; 29: 169-195. [CrossRef]
  55. Laukka SJ, Järvilehto T, Alexandrov YI, Lindqvist J. Frontal midline theta related to learning in a simulated driving task. Biol Psychol. 1995; 40: 313-320. [CrossRef]
  56. Sudsuang R, Chentanez V, Veluvan K. Effect of Buddhist meditation on serum cortisol and total protein levels, blood pressure, pulse rate, lung volume and reaction time. Physiol Behav. 1991; 50: 543-548. [CrossRef]
  57. Kaul P, Passafiume J, Sargent RC, O'Hara BF. Meditation acutely improves psychomotor vigilance, and may decrease sleep need. Behav Brain Funct. 2010; 6: 47.
  58. Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Nieuwenhuis S, Davidson RJ. Theta phase synchrony and conscious target perception: impact of intensive mental training. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009; 21: 1536-1549. [CrossRef]
  59. Johnson S, Gur RM, David Z, Currier E. One-session mindfulness meditation: a randomized controlled study of effects on cognition and mood. Mindfulness. 2015; 6: 88-98. [CrossRef]
  60. Valentine ER, Sweet PL. Meditation and attention: A comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Ment Health Relig Cult. 1999; 2: 59-70. [CrossRef]
  61. Anderson ND, Lau MA, Segal ZV, Bishop SR. Mindfulness‐based stress reduction and attentional control. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2007; 14: 449-463. [CrossRef]
  62. Melloni M, Sedeño L, Couto B, Reynoso M, Gelormini C, Favaloro R, et al. Preliminary evidence about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity and social cognition. Behav Brain Funct. 2013; 9: 47. [CrossRef]
  63. Kohler M, Rawlings M, Kaeding A, Banks S, Immink MA. Meditation is effective in reducing sleepiness and improving sustained attention following acute sleep restriction. J Cogn Enhanc. 2017; 1: 210-218. [CrossRef]
  64. Cahn BR, Delorme A, Polich J. Occipital gamma activation during vipassana meditation. Cogn Process. 2010; 11: 39-56. [CrossRef]
  65. Moore A, Malinowski P. Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious Cogn. 2009; 18: 176-186. [CrossRef]
  66. Teper R, Inzlicht M. Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: the importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012; 8: 85-92. [CrossRef]
  67. Hodgins HS, Adair KC. Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and cognition. 2010; 19: 872-878. [CrossRef]
  68. Hauswald A, Übelacker T, Leske S, Weisz N. What it means to be Zen: Marked modulations of local and interareal synchronization during open monitoring meditation. NeuroImage. 2015; 108: 265-273. [CrossRef]
  69. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Rawlings NB, Francis AD, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Mental training enhances attentional stability: neural and behavioral evidence. J Neurosci. 2009; 29: 13418-13427. [CrossRef]
  70. Hölzel BK, Carmody J, Vangel M, Congleton C, Yerramsetti SM, Gard T, et al. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2011; 191: 36-43. [CrossRef]
  71. Ferrarelli F, Smith R, Dentico D, Riedner BA, Zennig C, Benca RM, et al. Experienced mindfulness meditators exhibit higher parietal-occipital EEG gamma activity during NREM sleep. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e73417. [CrossRef]
  72. Braboszcz C, Cahn BR, Levy J, Fernandez M, Delorme A. Increased gamma brainwave amplitude compared to control in three different meditation traditions. PloS one. 2017; 12: e0170647. [CrossRef]
  73. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport. 2005; 16: 1893. [CrossRef]
  74. Kang D-H, Jo HJ, Jung WH, Kim SH, Jung Y-H, Choi C-H, et al. The effect of meditation on brain structure: cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012; 8: 27-33. [CrossRef]
  75. Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, Rosenkranz M, Muller D, Santorelli SF, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosom Med. 2003; 65: 564-570. [CrossRef]
  76. Pagnoni G, Cekic M. Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance in Zen meditation. Neurobiol Aging. 2007; 28: 1623-1627. [CrossRef]
  77. Mason LI, Alexander CN, Travis FT, Marsh G, Orme-Johnson D, Gackenbach J, et al. electrophysiological correlates of higher states of consciousness during sleep in long-term: practitioners of the transcendental meditation program. Sleep. 1997; 20: 102-110. [CrossRef]
  78. Sulekha S, Thennarasu K, Vedamurthachar A, Raju TR, Kutty BM. Evaluation of sleep architecture in practitioners of Sudarshan Kriya yoga and Vipassana meditation. Sleep and Biological Rhythms. 2006; 4: 207-214. [CrossRef]
  79. Pattanashetty R, Sathiamma S, Talakkad S, Nityananda P, Trichur R, Kutty BM. Practitioners of vipassana meditation exhibit enhanced slow wave sleep and REM sleep states across different age groups. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2010; 8: 34-41. [CrossRef]
  80. Dentico D, Ferrarelli F, Riedner BA, Smith R, Zennig C, Lutz A, et al. Short meditation trainings enhance non-REM sleep low-frequency oscillations. PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0148961. [CrossRef]
  81. Tooley GA, Armstrong SM, Norman TR, Sali A. Acute increases in night-time plasma melatonin levels following a period of meditation. Biol Psychol. 2000; 53: 69-78. [CrossRef]
  82. Harinath K, Malhotra AS, Pal K, Prasad R, Kumar R, Kain TC, et al. Effects of Hatha yoga and Omkar meditation on cardiorespiratory performance, psychologic profile, and melatonin secretion. J Altern Complement Med. 2004; 10: 261-268. [CrossRef]
  83. Aeschbach D, Borbely AA. All‐night dynamics of the human sleep EEG. J Sleep Res. 1993; 2: 70-81. [CrossRef]
  84. Scullin MK, Gao C. Dynamic contributions of slow wave sleep and REM sleep to cognitive longevity. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2018; 4: 284-293. [CrossRef]
  85. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep. 2004; 27: 1255-1273. [CrossRef]
  86. Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015; 10: 97-137. [CrossRef]
  87. Bliwise DL, Scullin MK. Normal Aging. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, eds. Principles and practices of sleep medicine. 6th ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2017. p. 25-38. [CrossRef]
  88. Richardson GS, Carskadon MA, Orav EJ, Dement WC. Circadian variations of sleep tendency in elderly and young adult subjects. Sleep. 1982; 5: 82-94. [CrossRef]
  89. Foley DJ, Monjan AA, Brown SL, Simonsick EM, Wallace RB, Blazer DG. Sleep complaints among elderly persons: an epidemiologic study of three communities. Sleep. 1995; 18: 425-432. [CrossRef]
  90. Högl B, Kiechl S, Willeit J, Saletu M, Frauscher B, Seppi K, Müller J, Rungger G, Gasperi A, Wenning G, Poewe W. Restless legs syndrome: a community-based study of prevalence, severity, and risk factors. Neurology. 2005; 64: 1920-1924. [CrossRef]
  91. Bliwise DL, Foley DJ, Vitiello MV, Ansari FP, Ancoli-Israel S, Walsh JK. Nocturia and disturbed sleep in the elderly. Sleep med. 2009; 10: 540-548. [CrossRef]
  92. Gong H, Ni CX, Liu YZ, Zhang Y, Su WJ, Lian YJ, Peng W, Jiang CL. Mindfulness meditation for insomnia: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychosom Res. 2016; 89: 1-6. [CrossRef]
  93. Innes KE, Selfe TK, Agarwal P, Williams K, Flack KL. Efficacy of an eight-week yoga intervention on symptoms of restless legs syndrome (RLS): A pilot study. J Altern Complement Med. 2013; 19: 527-535. [CrossRef]
  94. Bablas V, Yap K, Cunnington D, Swieca J, Greenwood KM. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for restless legs syndrome: A proof of concept trial. Mindfulness. 2016; 7: 396-408. [CrossRef]
  95. Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Akbaraly TN, Marmot MG, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Change in sleep duration and cognitive function: Findings from the Whitehall II Study. Sleep. 2011; 34: 565-573. [CrossRef]
  96. Lutz A, Greischar LL, Rawlings NB, Ricard M, Davidson RJ. Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. PNAS. 2004; 101: 16369-16373. [CrossRef]
  97. Travis F. The junction point model: A field model of waking, sleeping, and dreaming, relating dream witnessing, the waking/sleeping transition, and Transcendental Meditation in terms of a common psychophysiologic state. Dreaming. 1994; 4: 91. [CrossRef]
  98. Dorrian J, Rogers N, Dinges D, Kushida C. Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In: Sleep deprivation: Clinical issues, pharmacology and sleep loss effects. NY: Marcel Dekker, Inc.; 2005. p. 39-70. [CrossRef]
  99. Takizawa R, Nishimura Y, Yamasue H, Kasai K. Anxiety and performance: the disparate roles of prefrontal subregions under maintained psychological stress. Cerebral Cortex. 2013; 24: 1858-1866. [CrossRef]
  100. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007; 7: 336. [CrossRef]
  101. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers Individ Dif. 2011; 50: 955-960. [CrossRef]
  102. Elliman NA, Green MW, Rogers PJ, Finch GM. Processing-efficiency theory and the working-memory system: Impairments associated with sub-clinical anxiety. Pers Individ Dif. 1997; 23: 31-35. [CrossRef]
  103. Pacheco-Unguetti AP, Lupiáñez J, Acosta A. Atención y ansiedad: relaciones de la alerta y el control cognitivo con la ansiedad rasgo. Psicológica. 2009; 30: 1-25.
  104. Smith JC. Psychotherapeutic effects of transcendental meditation with controls for expectation of relief and daily sitting. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1976; 44: 630-637. [CrossRef]
  105. Mohan A, Sharma R, Bijlani RL. Effect of meditation on stress-induced changes in cognitive functions. J Altern Complement Med. 2011; 17: 207-212. [CrossRef]
  106. Burger KG, Lockhart JS. Meditation's effect on attentional efficiency, stress, and mindfulness characteristics of nursing students. J Nur Edu. 2017; 56: 430-434. [CrossRef]
  107. Dillbeck MC, Orme-Johnson DW. Physiological differences between transcendental meditation and rest. Am Psychol. 1987; 42: 879-881. [CrossRef]
  108. Tang Y-Y, Ma Y, Wang J, Fan Y, Feng S, Lu Q, et al. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007; 104: 17152-17156. [CrossRef]
  109. Singh Y, Sharma R, Talwar A. Immediate and long-term effects of meditation on acute stress reactivity, cognitive functions, and intelligence. Altern Ther Health Med. 2012; 18: 46-53.
  110. Lazar SW, Bush G, Gollub RL, Fricchione GL, Khalsa G, Benson H. Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport. 2000; 11: 1581-1585. [CrossRef]
  111. András H, Anna S, Montana X, Lanquart JP, Hubain P, Flamand M, et al. Egyéni különbségek az alvási makrostruktúrában: A szorongás, a depresszió, az öregedés és a nem hatása. Neuropsychopharmacol Hung. 2015; 17: 146-156.
  112. Elder C, Nidich S, Moriarty F, Nidich R. Effect of transcendental meditation on employee stress, depression, and burnout: a randomized controlled study. Perm J. 2014; 18: 19-23. [CrossRef]
  113. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Direk N, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Tiemeier H. 24‐hour activity rhythm and sleep disturbances in depression and anxiety: A population‐based study of middle‐aged and older persons. Depress Anxiety. 2015; 32: 684-692. [CrossRef]
  114. Nielsen L, Kaszniak AW. Awareness of subtle emotional feelings: a comparison of long-term meditators and nonmeditators. Emotion. 2006; 6: 392-405. [CrossRef]
  115. Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Damasio AR, Davidson RJ, Lutz A, Tranel D. Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology. 2008; 45: 671-677. [CrossRef]
  116. Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia. 2008; 46: 224-232. [CrossRef]
  117. Pace TW, Negi LT, Adame DD, Cole SP, Sivilli TI, Brown TD, et al. Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009; 34: 87-98. [CrossRef]
Newsletter
Download PDF Download Citation
0 0

TOP